Economic and Social Reports
Gender-related differences in the career advancement of women in Canada

by Zechuan Deng
Release date: September 22, 2021

DOI: https://doi.org/10.25318/36280001202100900002-eng

Skip to text

Text begins

Abstract

In Canada, the gender wage gap continues to exist (Statistics Canada 2021). In 2018, nearly two-thirds of the gap remained unexplained by standard factors such as level of education, job attributes, proportions of women and men in higher-paying occupations or industries, and demographics (Pelletier, Patterson and Moyser 2019). This points to a continued need for analysis in this area to better understand gender-based wage disparity, including gender-related differences in career advancement. Using new content developed in the 2016 General Social Survey: Canadians at Work and Home, this study investigates gender-related differences in career advancement that may impact women’s wages. The results suggest that women were at a disadvantage in terms of career advancement, compared with their male counterparts. Women were less likely than men to have training paid for by their employer in the past 12 months. Among those who received training, women were less likely than men to feel that training had made their job more secure or had improved their prospects for future employment.

Author

Zechuan Deng is with the Strategic Analysis, Publications and Training Division, Analytical Studies and Modelling Branch at Statistics Canada.

Acknowledgement

This study is funded by the Department for Women and Gender Equality.

Introduction

In Canada, women’s labour force participation rate was stagnant throughout the 1990s and increased five percentage points between the late 1990s and 2015 (Drolet, Uppal and LaRochelle-Côté 2016). In addition, women’s labour market participation in all occupations has increased significantly since 1985, including in occupations traditionally dominated by men (Graham and Krahn 2000). Today, women also have higher educational attainment in Canada, except at the doctoral level (McDaniel 2010 and Ferguson 2016). However, the gender-based gap in wages and other labour market outcomes continue to persist. In 2020, women in Canada earned $0.89 for every dollar earned by men annually (Statistics Canada 2021). They also continue to be under-represented in leadership and executive positions, occupying just 21.6% of board positions in Canada’s top 500 corporations in 2016 (Canadian Board Diversity Council 2016).

While part of the gender wage gap in Canada can be explained by factors such as job attributes, proportions of women and men in higher-paying occupations or industries, and demographics, nearly two-thirds of the gap in 2018 were still unexplained (Pelletier, Patterson and Moyser 2019).Note  Possible explanations for this portion include other factors such as the potential under-representation of women in high-paying firms or other gender-related differences. This points to a continued need for analysis in this area to better understand gender-based wage disparity.

For example, a recent study from the United States based on classroom experiments showed that women tend to show less ambitious behaviour at work than men when men are present in the classroom. In addition, this behaviour is observed more frequently among single women than their married counterparts (Bursztyn, Fujiwara and Pallais 2017).Note  This phenomenon, depending on the situation, could be an example of gender-related differences. There are many challenges in identifying and measuring these differences, as they are often not directly observed. For example, there are no current surveys or survey questions in Canada that directly collect data regarding the issue of gender-related differences. Besides, not all respondents may be aware of or understand if they are affected by these issues, as some of these differences could already be internally normalized as part of the respondent’s gender norms. This is why these gender-related differences are important issues that should be brought to the attention of policy makers to improve policies that seek to address the gender wage gap.

The previous study based on the U.S. labour market has shown that both women and men are equally likely to adopt proactive behaviours and strategies at the workplace, which are generally attributed to an ideal worker (Carter and Silva 2011).Note  However, men benefitted more than women when they adopted these strategies. Even when women used the same career advancement strategies, they advanced more slowly than their male counterparts, and their pay increased at a slower pace. In Canada, there is a lack of similar or relevant studies. To fill this gap, this article will extend the findings from Carter and Silva (2011). It will discuss the possible existence and measure the magnitude of gender-related differences in career advancement for women that may hinder women’s career progression, based on hypotheses constructed from quantitative measurements collected in a national survey. These gender-related differences related to career advancement could be key contributing factors and potential channels that lead to the current gender-based wage gap in Canada that was previously ignored.

Data

The data source used in this study is the 2016 General Social Survey: Canadians at Work and Home. The main objective of the survey was to take a comprehensive look at the way Canadians live by incorporating the realms of work, home, leisure and overall well-being. The target population includes all non-institutionalized persons 15 years of age and older, living in the 10 provinces of Canada.Note  The main population of interest is respondents who were working as paid employees at the time of the survey (n=8,820). The full sample size of the survey is approximately 20,000 individuals, of whom more than half are women.Note  The distribution between women and men across most of the demographic and geographic indicators are relatively close except for occupation type, which is expected.Note  The 2016 General Social Survey (GSS) included many brand new perception questions related to work ethic and career advancement, which were never asked before in any other cycles of the GSS or major social surveys in Canada and made this study possible.

Methods

For this study, the main methods of analysis are based on a combination of descriptive statistics analysis and regression models. The purpose of the regression model is to verify the robustness of the results found in the descriptive statistics. For the descriptive statistics analysis, the primary outcome variables are perceptions of various workplace behaviours and opinions, such as self-reported work ethic scores and questions related to the prospects for career advancement, which are analyzed for both women and men.Note Note  For the regression analysis, the variable of interest is sex. The main independent variables, or control variables, are selected based on existing gender-based literature related to career advancement (Afza and Newaz, 2008, Mustapa, Mutalib and Noor 2018), which include age, age squared, marital status, highest level of education, immigrant status, visible minority status, presence of children under 18, personal income (before tax), size of the workplace, occupations, union status and geographic control variables such as province of residence and census population centre identifier. The main dependent or outcome variables include self-reported work ethic scores, measurement on the prospects for career advancement, whether individuals received formal training paid for by employer in the past 12 months, whether the job is more secure because of training received in the past 12 months, and whether future employment prospects are better because of training received in the past 12 months.Note Note  In this study, the main outcome variables are all binary variables; therefore, a probability model (standard linear probability model, probit or logit model) is preferred to the standard linear model to estimate the conditional effect of sex. It is worth mentioning that the results from descriptive statistics and regression models do not explain the possible causal relationship between sex and career advancement. It is merely a two-way correlation that could subject to issues of endogeneity.

Based on the study of Carter and Silva (2011), the following hypotheses are constructed for the purpose of identifying the possible gender-related differences in career advancement for women in Canada. The main outcome variables are self-reported work ethic scores and whether the respondents’ current job offers good prospects for career advancement. The key hypotheses here are construed as follows which are based on the “all else equal” condition:

  1. All else equal, women had equal or higher self-reported work ethic scores than their male counterparts.
  2. All else equal, women were more likely to disagree that their current job offers good prospects for career advancement.
  3. All else equal, women were less likely to receive formal training paid for by their employer.
  4. All else equal, women were more likely to receive formal training paid for by themselves.
  5. All else equal, women were less likely to feel that their jobs are more secure because of training they received.
  6. All else equal, women were less likely to feel that their future employment prospects are better because of training.

If these hypotheses are not rejected, then it is fair to conclude that there are some forms of gender-related differences in career advancement that might prevent women from advancing their careers, despite having higher or equal self-reported work ethic scores compared with their male counterparts.

For this study, the general econometric model takes the following form:

Pr ( Y i = 1 | X i ) = Φ   ( β 0 + β 1 F e m a l e i   + C i + Υ I   +   Ψ P + u i ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacaqGqbGaaeOCaiaacIcacaWGzbWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaamyAaaWd aeqaaOWdbiabg2da9iaaigdacaqG8bGaamiwa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbi aadMgaa8aabeaak8qacaqGPaGaeyypa0JaeuOPdyKaaiiOaiaacIca cqaHYoGypaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaIWaaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaeq OSdi2damaaBaaaleaapeGaaGymaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaadAeacaWGLbGa amyBaiaadggacaWGSbGaamyza8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabe aak8qacaGGGcGaey4kaSIaam4qa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aa beaak8qacqGHRaWkcqqHLoqvpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGjbaapaqaba GcpeGaaiiOaiabgUcaRiaacckacqqHOoqwpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWG qbaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaamyDa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8 aabeaak8qacaGGPaaaaa@63DA@

With E( u i | x i )=0 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacaWGfbGaaiikaiaadwhapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqabaGc peGaaeiFaiaadIhapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaae ykaiabg2da9iaaicdaaaa@3FB3@

This is a probit model where the conditional expectation of the dependent binary variable given all relevant information X of each observation i is equal to the cumulative distribution function of the standard normal distribution defined as Φ MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacqqHMoGraaa@3791@ . After submitting the outcome variables to the general model, the specific econometric model takes the following forms:

Pr(High work ethic score8 or abov e i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_1 + β 1_1 Femal e i  + C 1i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 1i ) (1) Pr(Good prospects for career advancemen t i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_2 + β 1_2 Femal e i  + C 2i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 2i ) (2) Pr(Received training paid for by employe r i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_3 + β 1_3 Femal e i  + C 3i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 3i )  (3) Pr(Received training paid for by employe e i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_4 + β 1_4 Femal e i  + C 4i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 4i )  (4) Pr(Job is more secure because of trainin g i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_5 + β 1_5 Femal e i  + C 5i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 5 )  (5) Pr(Better future career because of trainin g i =1| X i )=Φ ( β 0_6 + β 1_6 Femal e i  + C 6i + Υ I  +  Ψ P + u 6i )  (6) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGceaqabeaaqaaaaa aaaaWdbiaabcfacaqGYbGaaiikaiaadIeacaWGPbGaam4zaiaadIga caGGGcGaam4Daiaad+gacaWGYbGaam4AaiaacckacaWGLbGaamiDai aadIgacaWGPbGaam4yaiaacckacaWGZbGaam4yaiaad+gacaWGYbGa amyzaiabgkHiTiaaiIdacaGGGcGaam4BaiaadkhacaGGGcGaamyyai aadkgacaWGVbGaamODaiaadwgapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqa baGcpeGaeyypa0JaaGymaiaabYhacaWGybWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaam yAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaabMcacqGH9aqpcqqHMoGrcaGGGcGaaiikaiab ek7aI9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaicdacaGGFbGaaGymaaWdaeqaaOWdbi abgUcaRiabek7aI9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaigdacaGGFbGaaGymaaWd aeqaaOWdbiaadAeacaWGLbGaamyBaiaadggacaWGSbGaamyza8aada WgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabeaak8qacaGGGcGaey4kaSIaam4qa8aa daWgaaWcbaWdbiaaigdacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaeuyPdu 1damaaBaaaleaapeGaamysaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacckacqGHRaWkcaGG GcGaeuiQdK1damaaBaaaleaapeGaamiuaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabgUcaRi aadwhapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaIXaGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacMca caWLjaGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaWLjaGaaiikaiaaigdacaGGPaaabaGaae iuaiaabkhacaGGOaGaam4raiaad+gacaWGVbGaamizaiaacckacaWG WbGaamOCaiaad+gacaWGZbGaamiCaiaadwgacaWGJbGaamiDaiaado hacaGGGcGaamOzaiaad+gacaWGYbGaaiiOaiaadogacaWGHbGaamOC aiaadwgacaWGLbGaamOCaiaacckacaWGHbGaamizaiaadAhacaWGHb GaamOBaiaadogacaWGLbGaamyBaiaadwgacaWGUbGaamiDa8aadaWg aaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabeaak8qacqGH9aqpcaaIXaGaaeiFaiaadI fapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaaeykaiabg2da9iab fA6agjaacckacaGGOaGaeqOSdi2damaaBaaaleaapeGaaGimaiaac+ facaaIYaaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaeqOSdi2damaaBaaaleaapeGa aGymaiaac+facaaIYaaapaqabaGcpeGaamOraiaadwgacaWGTbGaam yyaiaadYgacaWGLbWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaa cckacqGHRaWkcaWGdbWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaaGOmaiaadMgaa8aabe aak8qacqGHRaWkcqqHLoqvpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGjbaapaqabaGc peGaaiiOaiabgUcaRiaacckacqqHOoqwpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGqb aapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaamyDa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaikdacaWG PbaapaqabaGcpeGaaiykaiaaxMaacaWLjaGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaGGOa GaaGOmaiaacMcaaeaacaqGqbGaaeOCaiaacIcacaWGsbGaamyzaiaa dogacaWGLbGaamyAaiaadAhacaWGLbGaamizaiaacckacaWG0bGaam OCaiaadggacaWGPbGaamOBaiaadMgacaWGUbGaam4zaiaacckacaWG WbGaamyyaiaadMgacaWGKbGaaiiOaiaadAgacaWGVbGaamOCaiaacc kacaWGIbGaamyEaiaacckacaWGLbGaamyBaiaadchacaWGSbGaam4B aiaadMhacaWGLbGaamOCa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabeaak8 qacqGH9aqpcaaIXaGaaeiFaiaadIfapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaa paqabaGcpeGaaeykaiabg2da9iabfA6agjaacckacaGGOaGaeqOSdi 2damaaBaaaleaapeGaaGimaiaac+facaaIZaaapaqabaGcpeGaey4k aSIaeqOSdi2damaaBaaaleaapeGaaGymaiaac+facaaIZaaapaqaba GcpeGaamOraiaadwgacaWGTbGaamyyaiaadYgacaWGLbWdamaaBaaa leaapeGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacckacqGHRaWkcaWGdbWdamaaBa aaleaapeGaaG4maiaadMgaa8aabeaak8qacqGHRaWkcqqHLoqvpaWa aSbaaSqaa8qacaWGjbaapaqabaGcpeGaaiiOaiabgUcaRiaacckacq qHOoqwpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGqbaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaamyD a8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaiodacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaaiykaiaacc kacaWLjaGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaWLjaGaaiikaiaaiodacaGGPaaabaGa aeiuaiaabkhacaGGOaGaamOuaiaadwgacaWGJbGaamyzaiaadMgaca WG2bGaamyzaiaadsgacaGGGcGaamiDaiaadkhacaWGHbGaamyAaiaa d6gacaWGPbGaamOBaiaadEgacaGGGcGaamiCaiaadggacaWGPbGaam izaiaacckacaWGMbGaam4BaiaadkhacaGGGcGaamOyaiaadMhacaGG GcGaamyzaiaad2gacaWGWbGaamiBaiaad+gacaWG5bGaamyzaiaadw gapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaeyypa0JaaGymaiaa bYhacaWGybWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaabMcacq GH9aqpcqqHMoGrcaGGGcGaaiikaiabek7aI9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaa icdacaGGFbGaaGinaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabgUcaRiabek7aI9aadaWgaa WcbaWdbiaaigdacaGGFbGaaGinaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaadAeacaWGLbGa amyBaiaadggacaWGSbGaamyza8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabe aak8qacaGGGcGaey4kaSIaam4qa8aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaisdacaWG PbaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaeuyPdu1damaaBaaaleaapeGaamysaa WdaeqaaOWdbiaacckacqGHRaWkcaGGGcGaeuiQdK1damaaBaaaleaa peGaamiuaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabgUcaRiaadwhapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qaca aI0aGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacMcacaGGGcGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaWL jaGaaCzcaiaacIcacaaI0aGaaiykaaqaaiaabcfacaqGYbGaaiikai aadQeacaWGVbGaamOyaiaacckacaWGPbGaam4CaiaacckacaWGTbGa am4BaiaadkhacaWGLbGaaiiOaiaadohacaWGLbGaam4yaiaadwhaca WGYbGaamyzaiaacckacaWGIbGaamyzaiaadogacaWGHbGaamyDaiaa dohacaWGLbGaaiiOaiaad+gacaWGMbGaaiiOaiaadshacaWGYbGaam yyaiaadMgacaWGUbGaamyAaiaad6gacaWGNbWdamaaBaaaleaapeGa amyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabg2da9iaaigdacaqG8bGaamiwa8aadaWgaa WcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabeaak8qacaqGPaGaeyypa0JaeuOPdyKaaiiO aiaacIcacqaHYoGypaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaIWaGaai4xaiaaiwdaa8 aabeaak8qacqGHRaWkcqaHYoGypaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaIXaGaai4x aiaaiwdaa8aabeaak8qacaWGgbGaamyzaiaad2gacaWGHbGaamiBai aadwgapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaaiiOaiabgUca RiaadoeapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaI1aGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabgU caRiabfw6av9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadMeaa8aabeaak8qacaGGGcGa ey4kaSIaaiiOaiabfI6az9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaadcfaa8aabeaak8 qacqGHRaWkcaWG1bWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaaGynaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaa cMcacaGGGcGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaWLjaGaaCzcaiaacIcacaaI1aGaai ykaaqaaiaabcfacaqGYbGaaiikaiaadkeacaWGLbGaamiDaiaadsha caWGLbGaamOCaiaacckacaWGMbGaamyDaiaadshacaWG1bGaamOCai aadwgacaGGGcGaam4yaiaadggacaWGYbGaamyzaiaadwgacaWGYbGa aiiOaiaadkgacaWGLbGaam4yaiaadggacaWG1bGaam4Caiaadwgaca GGGcGaam4BaiaadAgacaGGGcGaamiDaiaadkhacaWGHbGaamyAaiaa d6gacaWGPbGaamOBaiaadEgapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGPbaapaqaba GcpeGaeyypa0JaaGymaiaabYhacaWGybWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaamyA aaWdaeqaaOWdbiaabMcacqGH9aqpcqqHMoGrcaGGGcGaaiikaiabek 7aI9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaicdacaGGFbGaaGOnaaWdaeqaaOWdbiab gUcaRiabek7aI9aadaWgaaWcbaWdbiaaigdacaGGFbGaaGOnaaWdae qaaOWdbiaadAeacaWGLbGaamyBaiaadggacaWGSbGaamyza8aadaWg aaWcbaWdbiaadMgaa8aabeaak8qacaGGGcGaey4kaSIaam4qa8aada WgaaWcbaWdbiaaiAdacaWGPbaapaqabaGcpeGaey4kaSIaeuyPdu1d amaaBaaaleaapeGaamysaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacckacqGHRaWkcaGGGc GaeuiQdK1damaaBaaaleaapeGaamiuaaWdaeqaaOWdbiabgUcaRiaa dwhapaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaaI2aGaamyAaaWdaeqaaOWdbiaacMcaca GGGcGaaCzcaiaaxMaacaWLjaGaaCzcaiaacIcacaaI2aGaaiykaaaa aa@5571@

Here the outcome variables “High work ethic score – 8 or above i,” “Good prospects for career advancement – disagree or strongly disagree i,” “Received training paid for by employer i,” “Received training paid for by employee i,”  “Job is more secure because of training i,” and “Better future employment prospects because of training i” are binary variables for individual i equal to 1 if these outcome variables are “true.” For instance, the outcome variable “High work ethic score – 8 or above i” is a binary variable equal to 1 if the respondent has a self-reported work ethic score of 8 or above (on a scale from 0 to 10) over the selected work ethic variables and 0 otherwise.Note  Similarly, “Good prospects for career advancement—disagree or strongly disagree i” is a binary variable equal to 1 if the respondent disagrees or strongly disagrees that their current job offers good prospects for career advancement and 0 otherwise. Similarly, the main variable of interest, “female,” is a binary variable equal to 1 if the respondent is a woman and 0 otherwise. This concept applies to all the other control variables. C i MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacaWGdbWdamaaBaaaleaapeGaamyAaaWdaeqaaaaa@3827@  is the vector of personal characteristics control variables, which include age, age squared, immigrant status, highest level of education, visible minority status, presence of children under 18, personal income group, size of workplace, occupations and union status.Note  Most of the control variables in this vector are binary measurement except for age which is a continuous variable that reflects the respondent’s age, and age squared, which captures the diminishing effect of age on the outcomes. Υ I MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacqqHLoqvpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGjbaapaqabaaaaa@38C7@  is the vector of population centre or rural area indicator, which includes four different categories (rural area is omitted in regression as the reference group). Ψ P MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aqatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaaeaaaaaaaaa8 qacqqHOoqwpaWaaSbaaSqaa8qacaWGqbaapaqabaaaaa@38D5@  is the region or province fixed effects vector which includes a total of 9 provinces in Canada (Ontario is omitted in the regression as the reference group). To better interpret the results, the coefficient of the variable of interest “Female” is converted into average marginal effects, which is the conditional probability (partial derivative) of being a woman and given that the outcome variable is “true,” while holding everything else (control variables) constant.

In this study, three different specifications of the probit model are tested. The first specification regresses the binary measurement of educational attainment and labour market outcome variables on sex directly, without any control variables. In the second specification, selected demographic indicators are added to the regression to control for personal characteristics differences. Finally, the third specification takes account of the region or province fixed effects and regresses the binary measurement of labour market outcome variables on sex and personal characteristics control variables to capture the complete picture. The main goal of using different specifications of the model is to test the robustness of the effect of being female on labour market outcomes.Note 

Results

Overall, women had slightly higher self-reported work ethic scores compared with men

Overall, among paid employees, women had higher self-reported average work ethic scores compared with men. This applies to each of the work ethic measurements selected and the combined work ethic score.Note  For instance, women not only show a stronger willingness than men to want to work hard, (76.4% vs. 71.7%) but also express higher levels of admiration for those who work hard (88.3% vs. 84.8%). A similar trend is also observed when using the average work ethic scores instead of the binary indicator as presented below.


Table 1A
Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores. The information is grouped by Have a scores of 8 or above for the following questions: “On a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 being 'completely disagree,' and 10 being 'completely agree,' how do you feel about the following statements?” (appearing as row headers), Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Have a scores of 8 or above for the following questions: “On a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 being 'completely disagree,' and 10 being 'completely agree,' how do you feel about the following statements?” Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
I am happiest when I work hard 71.7 76.4Note ***
I am willing to put in the extra effort to get the job done, even if I am not rewarded for it 71.3 78.0Note ***
I take pride in the work that I do 89.3 92.0Note **
The best reward from working is a sense of accomplishment 63.3 69.7Note ***
Admiration for those who work hard 84.8 88.3Note ***
Combined work ethic score 70.1 78.4Note ***

Table 1B
Average self-reported work ethic scores, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Average self-reported work ethic scores. The information is grouped by On a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 being “completely disagree,” and 10 being “completely agree,” how do you feel about the following statements? (appearing as row headers), Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
On a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 being “completely disagree,” and 10 being “completely agree,” how do you feel about the following statements? Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
I am happiest when I work hard 8.2 8.3Note **
I am willing to put in the extra effort to get the job done, even if I am not rewarded for it 8.1 8.4Note ***
I take pride in the work that I do 9.1 9.3Note **
The best reward from working is a sense of accomplishment 7.8 8.2Note ***
Admiration for those who work hard 8.9 9.1Note ***
Combined work ethic score 8.4 8.7Note ***

Similarly, when breaking down the combined work ethic score by selected demographic and job characteristics, women overall had higher average scores of work ethic compared with men. For instance, across the majority of the age groups, the highest level of education completed, immigrant status, marital status, presence of children and occupations, women were more likely to score eight or above on the combined work ethic, compared with men. Overall, these findings are consistent with previous studies based on the labour market in the U.S. and in Britain, where surveys indicate that women are found to work harder than men do (Gorman and Kmec 2007). Furthermore, according to the results presented in Table 2, women who were single and never married were less likely to have a high combined work ethic score compared with those that were married or living common-law (75.3%vs. 79.2%), while the differences among men were not significant. This is in line with the findings from Bursztyn, Fujiwara and Pallais (2017) based on U.S. data. On the other hand, no significant differences were found between immigrant status and the presence of children of women, which is in line with previous findings from Canada (Bauder 2006) and the U.S. (Korabik and Rosin 1995 & Metz 2005).


Table 2
Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores, by sex and selected demographic and job characteristics
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores. The information is grouped by Have a scores of 8 or above for the combined work ethic score (appearing as row headers), Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Have a scores of 8 or above for the combined work ethic score Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
Age group
15 to 24 68.6 72.0Table 2 Note §
25 to 44 (reference group) 69.6 79.1Note ***
45 to 64 70.7 79.4Note ***
65 and older 78.3Table 2 Note  89.0Note *Table 2 Note §§
Highest level of education completed
High school graduate or less or some postsecondary education 70.0 78.1Note **
Trades, community college, CEGEP, or university certificate below Bbachelor’s degree 70.8 80.7Note ***Table 2 Note §
Bachelor’s degree (reference group) 70.5 76.7Note *
Above bachelor’s degree 67.1 75.0Note *
Immigrant status
Immigrant 75.3Table 2 Note §§ 78.9
Non-immigrant (reference group) 68.7 78.3Note ***
Marital status
Married or living common-law (reference group) 71.6 79.2Note ***
Widowed, separated or divorced 70.7 82.9Note ***Table 2 Note 
Single, never married 66.6Table 2 Note § 75.3Note **Table 2 Note §
Presence of children under the age of 18
No (reference group) 69.8 78.4Note ***
Yes 70.8 78.5Note ***
Occupation
Management occupations 74.8 79.7
Business, finance, and administration occupations 70.7 79.4Note **
Natural and applied sciences and related occupations 61.3Table 2 Note § 75.1Note **
Health occupations 64.1 83.3Note **Table 2 Note §§
Occupations in education, law and social, community, and government services 73.3 79.1
Occupations in art, culture, recreation and sport 75.2 79.8
Sales and service occupations (reference group) 69.9 75.8Note *
Trades, transport and equipment operators and related occupations 73.8 76.4
Natural resources, agriculture and related product occupations 73.5 87.3
Occupations in manufacturing and utilities 65.5 71.8

When measured by prospects for career advancement, women’s likelihood of having a high work ethic score was also higher than men’s. For instance, among individuals who agree, neither agree nor disagree, disagree, or strongly disagree that their job offers good prospects for career advancement, women were more likely to score eight or above on the combined work ethic score, compared with men (82.3% vs. 72.9%, 70.8% vs. 64.1%, 77.2% vs. 55.1% and 72.0% vs. 52.5% respectively). This suggests that regardless of current jobs’ potential for future growth, which is a key element of job quality, women expressed a higher level of moral duty in working in terms of paid employment, compared with men.


Table 3
Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores, by sex and prospects for career advancement
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals with high self-reported work ethic scores. The information is grouped by Have a score of 8 or above for the combined work ethic score (appearing as row headers), Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Have a score of 8 or above for the combined work ethic score Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
To what extent do you agree or disagree with the following statements?
Your job offers good prospects for career advancement
Strongly agree 88.2 87.5
Agree 75.9 82.3Note ***
Neither agree nor disagree 64.1 70.8Note *
Disagree 55.1 77.2Note ***
Strongly disagree 52.5 72.0Note ***

Women were more likely to disagree that their current job offers good prospects for career advancement compared with men

While overall, women had higher self-reported work ethic scores than their male counterparts, they were less likely to agree that their current job offers good prospects for career advancement, compared with men. For instance, women were less likely to strongly agree that their job offers good prospects for career advancement compared with men (12.8% vs. 16.0%). On the other hand, women were more likely to disagree that their job offers good prospects for career advancement compared with their male counterparts (22.2% vs. 18.9%). This is consistent with previous studies based on the U.S. labour market, where there are significant lags in terms of career advancement for women (Carter and Silva 2011).


Table 4
Distribution of individuals on whether current job offers good prospects for career advancement, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals on whether current job offers good prospects for career advancement. The information is grouped by To what extent do you agree or disagree with the following statements? (appearing as row headers), Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
To what extent do you agree or disagree with the following statements? Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
Your job offers good prospects for career advancement
Strongly agree 16.0 12.8Note **
Agree 38.1 36.4
Neither agree nor disagree 20.0 21.1
Disagree 18.9 22.2Note **
Strongly disagree 6.9 7.5

Women were less likely than men to receive formal training paid for by their employer and more likely to receive training paid for by themselves compared with men 

In addition to prospects for career advancement, training is also an important factor that could affect future employment and, ultimately, wage differences.Note  A previous study based on the U.S. labour market suggests that the absence of learning, training and professional development opportunities for women is found to be an almost universal phenomenon (Davis 2012). In comparison, women in Canada were less likely than men to report that they had formal training paid for by their employer in the past 12 months (39.9% vs. 43.4%). On the other hand, women were more likely to say they had formal training paid for by themselves in the past 12 months, compared with their male counterparts (14.0% vs. 11.0%). This suggests that similar to the case of the U.S., women had additional financial burdens when it comes to skills and human capital accumulation after entering the labour market, which is a key factor in career advancement. This could be a result of differences in occupations and industry and the job requirements related to positions. However, the fact that women were more likely than men to have formal training paid for by themselves does not eliminate the possibility that women have had less paid training because they chose not to participate in such training  and therefore should be interpreted with caution. In other words, the lower likelihood of women having paid training might reflect (at least in part) women’s decisions not to take paid training, rather than a lower offer rate of paid training by employers.


Table 5
Distribution of individuals who received formal training either paid for by the employer or by the employee in the past 12 months, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals who received formal training either paid for by the employer or by the employee in the past 12 months Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
In the past 12 months, have you had formal training paid for by your employer?
Yes 43.4 39.9Note *
No 56.7 60.1Note *
In the past 12 months, have you had formal training paid for by yourself?
Yes 11.0 14.0Note **
No 89.0 86.0Note **

Women were less likely to feel that their job is more secure because of the training received compared with men

For those that received either informal or formal training at work, women were less likely than men to feel that their job is more secure because of the training received (56.3% vs. 61.6%). This suggests that, regardless of whether the training is paid for by employer or employee, women were still at a disadvantage when it comes to job security.


Table 6
Distribution of individuals who received informal or formal training in the past 12 months and job security, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals who received informal or formal training in the past 12 months and job security Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
Do you feel that your job is more secure because of your training?
Yes 61.6 56.3Note **
No 38.4 43.7Note **

Women were less likely to feel that the prospects for future employment are better because of the training received compared with men

In addition to job security, women were also less likely than men to feel that the prospects for future employment are better because of the training received (66.8% vs. 72.0%). This suggests that, in addition to job security, regardless of whether the training is paid for by employer or employee, women were also at a disadvantage when it comes to the prospects for future employment. In summary, among those that received some type of training in the past 12 months, women were less likely to feel that, compared with men, their job security and the prospects for future employment are better.


Table 7
Distribution of individuals who received informal or formal training in the past 12 months and prospects for future employment, by sex
Table summary
This table displays the results of Distribution of individuals who received informal or formal training in the past 12 months and prospects for future employment Sex, Male (reference group) and Female, calculated using percent units of measure (appearing as column headers).
Sex
Male (reference group) Female
percent
Do you feel that your prospects for future employment are better because of your training?
Yes 72.0 66.8Note ***
No 28.0 33.2Note ***

According to the results of the descriptive statistics presented above, among paid employees, given that women had an equal or higher work ethic score compared with men, they were more likely than men to disagree that their current job offers good prospects for career advancement. In addition, women were less likely to receive formal training paid for by their employer, compared with men. And for those who received some form of training in the past 12 months, women were less likely than men to feel that their job security and prospects for future employment have improved. In summary, the results of the descriptive statistics suggest the presence of gender-related differences in Canada that might be preventing women from advancing their career, despite having higher self-reported work ethic scores compared with men.

To ensure the results from the descriptive statistics in identifying gender-related difference are significant and robust, five separate multivariate regressions are estimated based on the hypotheses and probit regression models are presented above in the Method section.

Women were 8.1 percentage points more likely to have an average  work ethic score of 8 or above compared with men

As shown in Table 8, among all three model specifications, there is a positive correlation between the variable of interest “Female” and the outcome variable “Having an average  work ethic score of 8 or above” while holding everything else constant. For instance, based on the third model specification, being a woman increase the probability of scoring a work ethic score of 8 or above by 8.1 percentage points, compared with being a man. These results support the findings in the descriptive statistics presented above in Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3, where women had a work ethic score equal to or higher than their male counterparts. This conclusion holds when using the average work ethic score as the outcome variable rather than the binary indicator.Note 


Table 8
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of having a higher standard of self-reported work ethic scores (in a scale 0 to 10, scored 8 or above in average for selected work ethic measurement variables), equation (1)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of having a higher standard of self-reported work ethic scores (in a scale 0 to 10. The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient 0.087Note *** 0.083Note *** 0.081Note ***
Standard error (0.013) (0.015) (0.015)
Demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 8,246 8,246 8,246

No difference between men and women in their perception that job offers career advancement when accounting for demographic control variables

The regression results of equation (2) show a positive correlation between the variable of interest “Female” and the outcome variable “Disagreeing that the current job offers good prospects for career advancement” while holding everything else constant. However, after the introduction of the demographic control variables, the coefficient estimate of “Female” is no longer statistically significant. This means that besides sex, there are other important factors in the demographic control variables that are correlated with the outcome variable, including age, immigrant status, firm size, personal income and occupation. Contrary to the results presented in Table 4, these results suggest that there is no difference between men and women in their perception that job offers career advancement.


Table 9
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of disagreeing that the current job offers good prospects for career advancement (disagree or strongly disagree of the statement: “Your job offers good prospects for career advancement”), equation (2)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of disagreeing that the current job offers good prospects for career advancement (disagree or strongly disagree of the statement: “Your job offers good prospects for career advancement”). The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient 0.040Note ** 0.010 0.011
Standard error (0.013) (0.016) (0.016)
Other demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 8,246 8,246 8,246

All else equal, women were 3.8 percentage points less likely than men to have formal training paid for by their employer but were no longer more likely to have training paid by themselves

According to the regression results presented below for equation (3) in Table 10, for those that received training, there is a negative correlation between the variable of interest “Female” and the outcome variable “The probability of receiving formal training paid for by employer” while holding other factors constant. For instance, based on the third model specification, being a woman decreases the likelihood of having formal training paid for by employer by 3.8 percentage points, compared with being a man. These results support the first part of descriptive statistics presented above in Table 5, where women were less likely to have formal training paid for by their employer compared with men.

On the other hand, the regression results of equation (4) in Table 11 suggest that there are no differences between comparable men and women in the likelihood of having training paid by themselves. This means that the higher percentage of women having training paid by themselves observed in Table 5 reflects compositional effects, i.e. gender differences in socio-economic variables.


Table 10
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of receiving formal training paid for by employer in the past 12 months, equation (3)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of receiving formal training paid for by employer in the past 12 months. The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient -0.039Note ** -0.038Note * -0.038Note *
Standard error (0.015) (0.018) (0.018)
Demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 8,237 8,237 8,237

Table 11
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of receiving formal training paid for by employee in the past 12 months, equation (3)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of receiving formal training paid for by employee in the past 12 months. The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient 0.033Note ** 0.012 0.012
Standard error (0.010) (0.011) (0.011)
Demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 8,241 8,241 8,241

Women were 5.1 percentage points less likely than men to feel that their job is more secure because of training received in the past 12 months

As shown in Table 12, among all three model specifications, there is a negative correlation between the variable of interest “Female” and the outcome variable “The probability of feeling that job is more secure because of training” while holding everything else constant. For instance, according to the third model specification, among workers that received training in the past 12 months, women were 5.1 percentage points less likely to feel that their job security has improved, compared with men. These results support the descriptive statistics presented above in Table 6, where women were less likely to think that their job is more secure because of training. However, these results does not necessarily imply that training had a smaller effect for women on their actual job security and therefore should be interpreted with caution. In other words, women less optimistic views of the impact of training on their job security might be driven by other factors that are not observed in the data. For instance, it could reflect gender differences in the extent to which individuals are cautious when they assess the impact of training.


Table 12
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of feeling that job is more secure because of the training received in the past 12 months, equation (4)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of feeling that job is more secure because of the training received in the past 12 months. The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient -0.063Note *** -0.047Note ** -0.051Note **
Standard error (0.017) (0.019) (0.019)
Demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 5,736 5,736 5,736

Women were 3.7 percentage points less likely to feel that their prospects for future employment are better because of training received in the past 12 months, compared with men

According to the regression results presented below for equation (6), for those that received training, there is a negative correlation between the variable of interest “Female” and the outcome variable “The probability of feeling that the prospect for future employment is better because of training” while holding other factors constant. For instance, based on the third model specification, being a woman decreased the likelihood of agreeing that the prospect for future employment is better because of training received in the past 12 months by 3.7 percentage points, compared with being a man. These results support the descriptive statistics presented above in Table 7, where women were less likely to feel that their future employment is better because of training, compared with men. However, similar to the case of job security, these results do not necessarily imply that training had a smaller effect for women on their actual employment prospects, and therefore should be interpreted with caution. In other words, women’s less optimistic views of the impact of training on their employment prospects might be driven by other factors that are not observed in the data. For instance, it might reflect gender differences in the extent to which individuals are cautious when they assess the impact of training.


Table 13
Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of feeling that the prospect for future employment are better because of training received in the past 12 months, equation (5)
Table summary
This table displays the results of Probit model result: Average marginal effects of sex on the probability of feeling that the prospect for future employment are better because of training received in the past 12 months. The information is grouped by Variable (appearing as row headers), Marginal effects (1), Marginal effects (2) and Marginal effects (3) (appearing as column headers).
Variable Marginal effects (1) Marginal effects (2) Marginal effects (3)
Female
Coefficient -0.057Note *** -0.031Table 13 Note  -0.037Table 13 Note 
Standard error (0.016) (0.019) (0.018)
Demographic control variables No Yes Yes
Population centre control variables No No Yes
Region and province fixed effects No No Yes
Replications 500 500 500
Number of observations 5,726 5,726 5,726

In sum, these results are consistent with the gender-based trend found in the U.S. labour market, where women have a higher level of work ethic compared with men but, at the same time, show lags in terms of career advancement (Graham and Krahn, 2000, Carter and Silva 2011). Referring back hypothesis listed above in the Method section, the multivariate analyses suggest that all else equal, women are less likely than men to receive formal training paid for by their employer and to feel that this training makes their job more secure or improves their employment prospects. These results suggest that some gender-related differences in Canada might prevent women from advancing their careers, despite having higher self-reported work ethic scores compared with men.

As a robustness check, the logit model and odds ratios are estimated through equations (1) to (5), which showed consistent results.Note 

Limitations

The study has a few limitations. First, the variable that captures training paid by employers does not measure the degree to which employers offered paid training. It simply measures whether workers had paid training. The distinction is quite important: it is conceivable that women may have had less paid training not because employers offered them less paid training but because they might have chosen—for a variety of reasons—not to participate in such training. Whether this is the case or not is an empirical question that cannot be addressed with GSS 2016.

Second, the study cannot shed light on the reasons why women were less likely than men to feel that training had made their job more secure or had improved their prospects for future employment. Third, these results do not necessarily imply that training had a smaller effect on women’s actual job security or employment prospects. They might simply reflect the possibility that women might be more cautious than men when assessing training impacts.

Lastly, it is also worth mentioning that, although GSS cycles include an indicator of Indigenous identity, data are not available for First Nations people living on or off reserve specifically. Using a telephone-based sample design, it may be possible that First Nations and non-Indigenous people living on reserve (with Internet or telephone service) are selected as respondents. However, the number of persons residing on reserve who are interviewed has not, to date, been large enough to enable reliable estimates for the First Nations people living on reserve. Depending on the sample size of each GSS cycle and the type of output analysis required, data for First Nations people, Métis and Inuit must often be aggregated to the total Indigenous population (by combining the three Indigenous identity groups) to obtain population counts high enough to be reliable for publication, which is the case for the 2016 GSS. In this study, due to the issue of low counts, Indigenous identity is not included in the regression analysis.

Conclusion

Using new content developed in the 2016 General Social Survey: Canadians at Work and Home, this study investigates the possible existence and magnitude of gender-related differences in career advancement that might prevent women from advancing in their careers.

Results from this paper suggest that women were at a disadvantage in terms of career advancement, compared with their male counterparts. Women were less likely than men to have training paid for by their employer in the past 12 months. Among those who received training, women were less likely than men to feel that training had made their job more secure or had improved  their prospects for future employment.

These results serve as preliminary evidence suggesting that there could be some forms of gender-related differences in career advancement in Canada that might hinder women’s career progression. These differences could be contributing factors to the persistent gender-based wage gap, other than the standard explanatory factors (e.g. level of education, job attributes, proportions of women and men in higher-paying occupations or industries, and demographics). However, in order to reach a more definitive conclusion, an alternative dataset and more specific measurements on the issue of gender-related difference with greater details on job and employer characteristics are needed. Perhaps a specialized topic of General Social Survey or brand new harmonized contents designed around this issue would be ideal.

References

Afza, S.R., and M.K. Newaz. 2008. “Factors determine the presence of glass ceiling and influencing women career advancement in Bangladesh.” BRAC University Journal V (1): 85–92.

Bauder, H. 2006. “Origin, Employment Status and Attitudes towards Work: Immigrants in Vancouver, Canada.” Work, Employment and Society 20 (4): 709–729.

Bursztyn, L., T. Fujiwara, and A. Pallais. 2017. “Acting Wife”: Marriage Market Incentive and Labour Market Investments. National Bureau of Economic Research, Working Paper Series, no. 23043. Cambridge, Massachusetts: National Bureau of Economic Research.

Carter, N.M., and C. Silva. 2011. The Myth of The Ideal Worker: Does Doing All the Right Things Really Get Women Ahead. Toronto: Catalyst, Catalyst Publication Code: D108.

Canadian Board Diversity Council. 2017. Canadian Board Diversity Council 2016 Report Card. Toronto: Canadian Board Diversity Council.

Davis, P.J. 2012. The global training deficit: the scarcity of formal and informal professional development opportunities for women entrepreneurs. Industrial and Commercial Training 44 (1) 19–25.

Drolet, M., S. Uppal, and S. LaRochelle-Côté. 2016. The Canada-U.S. gap in women’s labour market participation. Insights on Canadian Society, August. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 75-006-X. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.

Ferguson, S. 2016. “Women and Education: Qualifications, Skills and Technology.” Women in Canada: A Gender-based Statistical Report. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 89-503-X. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.

Gorman E.H., and A.J. Kmec. 2007. “We (have to) try harder: Gender and Required Work Effort in Britain and the United States.” Gender & Society 21: 828–856.

Helliwell, J.F., G. Schellenberg and J. Fonberg. 2020. “The COVID-19 pandemic and life satisfaction in Canada.” StatCan COVID-19: Data to Insights for a Better Canada. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 45-28-0001. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.

Korabik, K. and H.M. Rosin. 1995. “The Impact of Children on Women Managers’ Career Behavior and Organizational Commitment.” Human Resources Management 34 513–528

McDaniel, A. 2010. “Cross-national Gender Gaps in Educational Expectations: The Influence of National-Level Gender Ideology and Educational Systems.” Comparative Education Review 54 (1): 27–50.

Metz, I. 2005. “Advancing the Careers of Women with Children.” Career Development International 10 (3) 228–245.

Mustapa, N. S., M.A. Mutalib, and K.M. Noor. 2018. “Why Can’t We Have Both? A Discussion on Work-Life Balance and Women Career Advancement in Malaysia.” Journal of Asian Finance, Economics and Business 5 (3) 103–112.

Pelletier, R., M. Patterson, and M. Moyser. 2019. The gender wage gap in Canada: 1998 to 2018. Labour Statistics: Research Papers. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 75-004-M2019004. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.

Statistics Canada. Table 14-10-0340-01 Employee wages by occupation, annual (table). Last updated: June 14, 2021.

Date modified: